Questions and
Answers
Question # 41 FONT>
What are the relationships among NLD,
Asperger's Syndrome (AS), and High Functioning Autism
(HFA)?
(1) Virtually all persons with AS
exhibit Definite or Probable NLD (See Question #10 for
characterizations of Definite and Probable NLD).
(2) No
persons with HFA exhibit Definite or Probable NLD.
These
conclusions are based upon extensive research on this topic (See the AS
section of NLD and
Neurological Disease for these references.)
Below are data that
can be used to constitute a (hopefully) straightforward demonstration of
conclusions (1) and (2). (See NLD Content &
Dynamics for NLD Assets and Deficits.)
SOME PRINCIPAL NLD
ASSETS & DEFICITS (14-YEAR-OLD; yes=Deficit no=No Deficit)
Neuropsychological & Neurolinguistic Assets
| |
|
|
AS |
HFA |
|
auditory perception |
no |
no |
|
auditory attention |
no |
??? |
auditory memory |
no |
??? |
|
language output |
no |
yes |
language assoc. |
no |
yes |
language form |
no |
yes |
Neuropsychological & Neurolinguistic
Deficits
| |
|
somatosensory |
??? |
??? |
psychomotor |
yes |
no |
visual-spatial |
yes |
no |
deal with novelty |
yes |
yes |
|
tactile attention |
yes |
??? |
visual attention |
yes |
no |
|
exploration |
yes |
no |
|
tactile memory |
yes |
??? |
visual memory |
yes |
??? |
|
concept-formation |
yes |
no |
problem-solving |
yes |
no |
|
language content |
yes |
??? |
language function |
yes |
??? | ............................................................................
Neuropsychological & Neurolinguistic
| |
Assets and Deficits: 2 Summaries
| |
|
1. ASSETS & DEFICITS
| |
|
(yes= exhibited; no= not exhibited)
| |
|
ASSETS
| |
|
|
|
YES |
NO |
??? |
|
AS |
6 |
0 |
0 |
|
HFA |
1 |
3 |
2 |
|
DEFICITS
| |
|
|
|
YES |
NO |
??? |
|
AS |
12 |
0 |
1 |
|
HFA |
1 |
6 |
6 |
|
|
|
2. POSITIVE (+) AND NEGATIVE (-) NLD INDICATORS
| |
|
|
|
|
+ |
- |
??? |
|
AS |
18 |
0 |
1 |
|
HFA |
2 |
8 |
9 |
|
|
|
| Observations.
(1)
These data and summaries would certainly be consistent with the
Conclusions enunciated above. This may be especially apparent in (2) above
that summarizes the positive and negative characteristics of NLD in AS and
HFA.
(2) These Conclusions would support the notion that AS and
HFA do no not lie on some sort of "spectrum." Indeed, the so-called
"Autistic Spectrum" may not include AS.
(3) One important
limitation of this (informal) study is that there are too many ???,
especially for HFA. Additional research should rectify this situation.
(4) The Academic and Psychosocial Assets of AS and HFA are
characterized in terms of their similarity to NLD (yes= deficit; no= no
deficit):
|
AS |
HFA |
|
Academic Assets |
|
read single words |
no |
yes |
|
spell words |
no |
yes |
|
Academic Deficits |
|
reading comp. |
yes |
??? |
arithmetic |
yes |
yes |
............................................................................ |
Psychosocial Asset |
|
social intent |
no |
yes |
|
Psychosocial Deficits |
|
social perception |
yes |
yes |
|
social competence |
yes |
yes |
|
intnlzd. psypathol. |
yes |
??? |
|
............................................................................ |
|
|
|
| These data are too incomplete
to allow for confident generalizations and conclusions.
However,
it is notable that the psychosocial deficits of AS and HFA are quite
similar. That said, it would seem highly probable that the sources of
these deficits are quite distinct, and have much to do with the fairly
long time that persons with AS (and NLD) attempt to establish social
contacts, as opposed to the reluctance of persons with HFA ever to try.
Thus, the psychosocial deficits of persons with AS (and NLD) may very well
be the direct result of their continuing, but failing, attempts at
socializing. Whereas, the deficits of those with HFA are the direct result
of not trying to perform in these areas. A similar difference in the
etiology of social perception deficits would also seem very likely.
Bottom Line. NLD would appear to constitute a "final common
pathway" for AS, as it does for many types of neurological disease,
disorder, and dysfunction (see NLD and
Neurological Disease); it is clear that HFA does not. That is, the
behavioural phenotype of AS is very similar to that evident in disorders
such as Williams syndrome, early shunted hydrocephalus, Triple X syndrome,
Sotos syndrome, and significant damage/lesions confined to the right
cerebral hemisphere; the behavioural phenotype of HFA has very little in
common with these and all other neurological diseases and disorders in
which NLD is clearly evident in their behavioural phenotypes. It would be
of interest to carry out analyses similar to those contained herein to
determine the extent to which the NLD syndrome is evident in their
phenotypes. Among other things, such analyses would allow for a "ranking"
of these diseases that may reveal interesting "clusters," such as those of
various genetic etiologies.
............................................................................
What are some of the important therapeutic implications of the
differences between HFA and those neurological disorders such as AS whose
behavioural phenotypes include NLD?
It is clear that behavioural
techniques involving the systematic application of rewards and
consequences have achieved quite positive results for many, if not the
majority of, youngsters with autism.
Such regimens are clearly
inappropriate for children who exhibit NLD (see Question #21), including
those diagnosed with AS. Insight-oriented dynamic psychotherapy is also
counterproductive (Question #23). Virtually
all therapeutic approaches to NLD that have been shown to have some
success involve intensive concentration on skill learning and anxiety
reduction, which are intimately related (see Question #16).
It
is clear that treatment plans for persons who exhibit NLD need to be
highly individualized (Question #31), and that
any system that ignores this (e.g., one that relies on rewards and
consequences) would be expected to be, at best, a failure and, at worst,
counterproductive. Dynamic psychotherapy for youngsters with NLD and their
families can, and often does, lead to very negative consequences (e.g., Question #20). The
individualization of treatment plans for adults must also take into
consideration their particular needs (Question
#40).
The neurodevelopmental unfolding of NLD is quite
predictable, especially with respect to psychosocial dynamics (Question #18), transient
psychosocial “comorbidities” (Question # 7), and those
of a more entrenched and serious nature
(Question #33, #38).
Bottom line. This very brief overview of the therapeutic needs of persons
with HFA versus those who exhibit NLD (e.g., those with AS) would suggest
strongly that making the distinctions suggested above between and among
these disorders is very helpful, indeed necessary, if therapeutic gains
are to be made. As was stated above with respect to the enormous
differences extant between persons with HFA and those with AS, it is clear
that they do not belong on any sort of therapeutic continuum or spectrum.
Their intervention needs appear as orthogonal to one another as are their
patterns of neuropsychological assets and deficits. Maintaining otherwise
will simply turn out to be counterproductive for achieving any significant
degree of adaptive development for these persons.
Final Note. Many students of AS and NLD seem to think that they are one
and the same. Of course, they are not. Reflections on the relevant sections
above and the NLD
and Neurological Disease section will show this assertion of
identity to be absurd.
Perhaps a little example from Aristotle’s Logic will be of assistance within this context.
Consider the following non-sequitur:
All apples are fruit.
Therefore, all fruit are apples.
Applied to the current considerations:
All persons with AS exhibit NLD.
Therefore, all persons with NLD have AS.
I rest my case.
References
Ellis, H. D. (1994). A preliminary study of right-hemisphere cognitive
deficits and impaired social judgment among young people with Asperger
syndrome. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
3, 255-266.
Ellis, H. D., & Gunter, H. L. (1999). Asperger syndrome: A simple
matter of white matter? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3,
192-200.
Gunter, H. L., Ghaziuddin, M., & Ellis, H. D. (2002). Asperger
syndrome: Tests of right hemisphere functioning and interhemispheric
communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
32, 263-281.
Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Volkmar, F., Cicchetti, D. V., & Rourke,
B. P. (1995). Asperger syndrome. In B. P. Rourke (Ed.), Syndrome of
nonverbal learning disabilities: Neurodevelopmental manifestations
(pp. 93-118). New York: Guilford Press.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., &
Rourke, B. P. (1995). Validity and neuropsychological characterization of
Asperger syndrome: Convergence with Nonverbal Learning Disabilities
syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36,
1127-1140.
Lincoln, A. et al. (1998). Neurobiology of Asperger syndrome: Seven
case studies and quantitative magnetic responance imaging findings. In E.
Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, & L. J. Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome or
high functioning autism? (pp.145-160). New York, Plenum.
McKelvey, J. R., Lambert, R., Mottron, L., & Shevell, M.I. (1995).
Right-hemisphere dysfunction in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child
Neurology, 10, 310-314.
Molina, J. L., Ruata, J. M., & Soler, E. P. (1986). Is there a
right-hemisphere dysfunction in Asperger’s syndrome? British Journal of
Psychiatry, 148, 745-746.
Nass, R., & Gutman, R. (1997). Boys with Asperger’s disorder,
exceptional verbal intelligence, tics, and clumsiness. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology, 39, 691-695.
Rourke, B. P., & Tsatsanis, K. D. (2000). Syndrome of Nonverbal
Learning Disabilities and Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. Volkmar, &
S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 231-253). New York:
Guilford Press.
Stevens, D. E., & Moffitt, T. E. (1988). Neuropsychological profile
of an Asperger's syndrome case with exceptional calculating ability.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2, 228-238.
Tsatsanis, K.D. (2003). Outcome research in Asperger syndrome and
autism. Child & Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
America, 12(1), 47-63.
Tsatsanis, K.D. (2004). Heterogeneity in learning style in Asperger
syndrome and high-functioning autism. Topics in Language Disorders,
24(4), 260-270.
Tsatsanis, K.D., Foley, C., & Donehower, C. (2004). Contemporary
outcome research and programming guidelines for Asperger syndrome and
high-functioning autism. Topics in Language Disorders,
24(4), 249-259.
Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1991). Nonautistic pervasive
developmental disorders. Psychiatry, 2, 1-12.
Witt, J. N. (1996). Diagnostic issues in the neuropsychological and
neurobehavioral assessment of a 12-year-old boy seen following epilepsy
surgery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11,
465-466.
NOTE:
THE INTERESTED READER MAY ALSO WISH TO
CONSULT THE FOLLOWING FOR CASE STRUDIES THAT ILLUSTRATE TYPICAL FINDINGS
IN YOUNGSTERS WITH MANY TYPES OF NEUROLOGICAL DISEASE DISORDER, AND
DYSFUNCTION, INCLUDING AS, WHEREIN THE SYNDROME OF NLD FORMS A SIGNIFICANT
PART OF THEIR BEHAVIOURAL PHENOTYPE:
Rourke, B. P., van der Vlugt,
H., & Rourke, S. B. (2002). Practice of child-clinical
neuropsychology: An introduction. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
|